Thank you to the British Academy for awarding
John Duncan and myself research funds to test a key hypothesis in the cognitive
neuroscience of human performance: is prefrontal cortex necessary for fluid
intelligence?
We will use non-invasive brain stimulation
(transcranial magnetic stimulation: TMS) to temporarily ‘deactivate’ the
prefrontal cortex, and then measure the consequences for performance on
standard tests of fluid intelligence. It is a relatively simple experimental
design, but if done correctly, the results should provide important and novel
insights into the brain mechanisms underlying one of the most important human
faculties: flexible reasoning and problem solving.
My co-investigator, John
Duncan, gained his reputation in the cognitive neuroscience of intelligence with
his seminal brain imaging study published in Science. This research
demonstrated that when people perform tasks that tax fluid intelligence, neural
activity increases in the prefrontal cortex relative to control tasks that
require less fluid intelligence.
This
result suggests that the prefrontal cortex is involved in fluid intelligence -
but of course, as every undergraduate in psychology/cognitive neuroscience
should be able to tell you, brain imaging alone cannot tell us whether the
activated brain area is in fact necessary for performing the task.
So,
to verify the causal role of the prefrontal cortex, Duncan and colleagues next
examined stroke patients (published in PNAS). The logic here is simple: does damage to the prefrontal
cortex reduce fluid intelligence? But the methodology is not so simple. Of
particular importance, how can you tell
whether a patient has low IQ because of the brain damage, or whether they were always a low IQ
individual?
Duncan's team tackled this problem by estimated pre-damage
fluid intelligence from scores on other tests that measure so-called
crystallised intelligence (e.g., vocabulary and general knowledge). Critically,
crystallised intelligence reflects the life long achievements that depend on
fluid intelligence during acquisition, and therefore can be used to approximate
pre-damage fluid intelligence. If the prefrontal cortex is especially important
for fluid intelligence, then damage should result in a disparity between fluid
and crystallised intelligence. Indeed, this is what they found.
As
developed in his popular science book, "How Intelligence Happens",
Duncan suggests that the prefrontal cortex is essential for flexible
structured cognitive processing, a key ingredient to fluid intelligence.
If this theory is correct, then temporary deactivation of the prefrontal cortex
should impair fluid intelligence. If not, then we need to rethink this working
hypothesis.
What
will these results tell us? Are we just heading back to 19th Century phrenology
– associating discrete brain areas with complex high order human traits that
are more like sociocultural inventions than principled neurocognitive
constructs? Do we then plan to localise creativity here, insight there, and perhaps
a little bit of moral judgment over here?
However, we must also recognise that we have to be careful how these results could be interpreted. Intelligence is a particularly sensitive area. The very concept of fluid intelligence often takes on more than it should - a reflection of the fundamental worth or even moral character of the individual.
Obviously there is some danger in reducing one of the most
important cognitive mechanisms to a single number (e.g., intelligence quotient:
IQ), which we can then compare between individuals and against groups. It is a
dangerous business that can be exploited for any number of nefarious agendas. For
example, we can try to confirm our own racial or sexist prejudices, conjuring
up a biological, and therefore 'scientific' excuse for beliefs that are
motivated by simple bigotry (recall the recent Watson controversy?).
Conversely, on the other side, the same logic could be used to pursue an
equality agenda. This could also be a dangerous path to follow - what if we are
not all equal in ability? I see no a priori reason that there should not be
group differences in any measure, including IQ. It is simply an empirical
issue, and therefore a risky business to stake our sense of equality on equal
ability.
IQ
is certainly a loaded concept. Recently, I was speaking with a mathematician
and historian about an advert they saw for a brain imaging study comparing IQ
between academics from the sciences and humanities. The historian was
intrigued, and eager to participate, whereas the mathematician was much more
reluctant. I guess the risk of a lower-than-hoped-for score is quite
disconcerting when your very livelihood depends on an almost
mythical concept of pure intelligence, or better still - genius.
An anecdote comes to mind of a researcher who was to be the first subject in an fMRI study of IQ conducted by his colleagues. Being scanned can sometimes make people nervous the first time, but this was a seasoned neuroscientist, no stranger to the confined and uncomfortable space of an MRI. Rather, what made this no ordinary scanning experience was the fact that his respected colleagues were watching from the control room, monitoring his responses to the IQ task. Enough to make any academic uncomfortable!
This
kind of awkwardness raises an important practical issue for us. Like many cognitive neuroscientists, I often rely on friends and colleagues to
participate in my experiments, especially students, academic visitors, post-doctoral
researchers. Obviously, one could easily imagine some tension arising in a lab
that has tested everyone’s IQ. This could be particularly worrying for the more
senior amongst us, as fluid intelligence is negatively correlated with age. We
would not want to upset the natural order of the academic hierarchy!
Anyway, I will keep you posted how we get on with the
project.
No comments:
Post a Comment